News
The latest research and innovations in the fields of depression and bipolar disorders.
The latest research and innovations in the fields of depression and bipolar disorders.
Drs. Fitzgerald and Rosenblum are leading an NIMH-funded multi-site trial of an innovative intervention for early childhood anxiety. Dr. Fitzgerald is quoted in this story about their research. This article was originally published by AIM Youth Mental Health.
*AIM Youth Mental Health is a non-profit dedicated to finding and funding promising youth mental health research that can identify solutions to make a difference in young people’s lives today. The organization contributed to funding Dr. Fitzgerald’s research. Read the original article here.*
By Jill Suttie
Anxiety is one of the most common childhood mental disorders. About 7% of children suffer from it at any given time, with nearly 1 in 3 adolescents experiencing it sometime during their teen years.
For an anxious child, seemingly normal activities can be hard. Worried kids have trouble adjusting to school, making friends, and learning. They can feel inhibited, avoiding challenges by running away or retreating into themselves. While parents may feel desperate to help, their approaches can backfire, inadvertently making their kids’ anxiety worse.
To help anxious kids, clinicians have developed science-based treatments, like cognitive-behavioral therapy, to alleviate symptoms. But, the treatments can be cumbersome and expensive, and they don’t always work—especially in very young children. And, while early signs of anxiety in kids as young as pre-school-aged can be a can be a sign of future trouble—a precursor to later disorders, like social anxiety, phobias, or obsessive compulsive disorder—less is known about how to stop anxiety in its tracks at very young ages, when kids may not even have the cognitive capacity to benefit from the treatment.
What if very young kids could be inoculated against anxiety somehow, sparing them from a future of worry and inhibition? Now, a new line of research conducted by Kate Fitzgerald, professor of Psychiatry and Obstetrics at the University of Michigan, suggests this may be possible.
With funding from the AIM Youth Mental Health Foundation, Fitzgerald has been studying very young children with anxious symptoms and making important discoveries about the neural markers for childhood anxiety. Building on this work, she and her team have created a training program for young children aimed at increasing their cognitive capacities, helping to lessen their anxiety—both immediately and, possibly, in the future.
“We hope our work will show that childhood anxiety is not inevitable, but might be prevented with the right intervention,” says Fitzgerald. “So far, it’s looking promising.”
When people face challenging or scary situations in life, our brains naturally go into action. The amygdala sends out neurochemicals (like adrenaline) to make our hearts pound and prepare our bodies to “fight, flight, or freeze” in case of danger. At the same time, the frontal lobes engage our cognition to assess the situation, draw from past experience, and problem-solve to come up with an appropriate response. In healthy people, these dual systems work in tandem—one putting on the gas and the other applying the brakes—depending on what’s needed.
Anxiety can have a positive side—like, when it motivates us to practice hard to master a piano piece or study for a test. But, in anxious people, it can be debilitating, as that gas pedal goes to the metal every time, making anxious people want to run or flee challenge. It can be exhausting, too, as they often have to exert a lot of effortful control just to get through. Facing stressful situations while tamping down that fear response is key to overcoming anxiety—in adults as well as older kids.
But, is this process in the brain the same for young kids? Interestingly, Fitzgerald and her team have been making some groundbreaking discoveries, showing how anxious young kids respond a little differently. For example, they have a higher than normal startle response in “neutral situations”—where nothing threatening is happening—but have a normal startle response in scary situations that any child might react to. That suggests that they have more to overcome when facing everyday challenges, like going to school or meeting new people.
They’ve also discovered that a part of the brain that responds when people make a mistake—the error-related negativity (or ERN)—is weaker in anxious 5-7-year olds than in worried older children and adults. That’s likely because young kids don’t have well developed cognitive capacities that could help them understand that errors happen, aren’t scary, and can often be fixed. Without more cognitive control, their startle response wins out, making them anxious, says Fitzgerald.
Interestingly, a child with low cognitive control in younger years is also more likely to develop childhood anxiety later on, while one with a higher capacity will be more resilient to stress. Raising cognitive control (which can be measured by the ERN) could both treat anxiety in young children and prevent it from becoming worse over time.
“If we could just help kids gain some cognitive control when they are anxious, it could really make a difference in how they deal with stressful situations,” says Fitzgerald. “We just need to empower them.”
To test this idea, Fitzgerald and her colleagues conducted a pilot study (unpublished) with anxious 4-7-year-olds. The children came to a “camp” the researchers designed called Kid Power for four half-day sessions over a two-week period. At the camp, children played fun, ordinary childhood games, like “Simon Says” and “Red Light/Green Light”, that help strengthen cognitive control.
Counselors at the camp gradually increased the challenge within the games to help kids master the skills needed to do well—like, being flexible, using their working memory, and inhibiting undesirable responses (like moving when they’re supposed to freeze). They also enjoyed the company of other kids, with whom they brainstormed ways to improve their performance. And, parents participated at the end of each session, learning the games from their kids so they could practice playing together at home.
To see the effects this training had on the kids’ brains and behavior, Fitzgerald and colleagues brought them into the lab and measured their startle response and ERN before they attended the Kid Power camp and 4-6 weeks after. To do that, they had kids play computer games that required cognitive control, while wearing special monitors that could capture their startle and ERN responses when mistakes were made. Additionally, they gathered information from the parents and the kids themselves about anxiety symptoms before and after the camp.
After analyzing the data, the team found that the children’s ERN’s were higher (signifying greater cognitive control), while their startle responses went down—a pattern associated with less anxiety at that age.
“The brain signal that related to detecting an error actually increased, but in a good way,” said Fitzgerald. “Kids were getting better at doing hard things, stopping instinctual responding, including the fear response.”
This mirrored the children’s (and their parents’) own assessments. They reported fewer anxiety symptoms, including fear and avoidance behavior, after the training—something Fitzgerald found particularly rewarding.
“It’s exciting to link the brain to behavior, but what’s even more rewarding is the individual children we’ve seen go through the program who are experiencing less anxiety symptoms,” she says.
For example, one parent reported that her daughter, who’d had symptoms of OCD prior to attending the Kid Power camp, had made noticeable improvement, even while the camp was still going on.
“She didn’t want to leave while she was here, and she was in a better mood during the week in between— a little less rigid and able to experience more joy,” the parent wrote in an evaluation.
Fitzgerald recalls another 5-year-old camper who’d been very afraid of making mistakes in his Kindergarten class, which led to bouts of crying and other disruptive behaviors requiring daily calls home. After attending the camp, though, and learning how to calm anxiety, everything changed.
“After a week of playing those games that were part of the intervention, those calls from home stopped,” says Fitzgerald. “His mom was impressed, because earlier counseling with a trained therapist had not led to improvement. Only after Kid Power did he successfully adjust to kindergarten and begin to enjoy it.”
With encouraging results from this pilot study, Fitzgerald applied for and received a $3 million National Institute of Health grant to expand the Kid Power program and conduct further research. She hopes future studies will help her nail down the key ingredient in the intervention that led to reduced anxiety and, potentially, find a way to tailor treatment to individual children—some of whom may need a stronger dose of the training or a slightly different intervention to improve, she says.
If her initial findings hold, her work could have broad implications, providing an easily replicated intervention for treating and preventing childhood anxiety disorders in the future.
“Interventions are within reach,” she says. “As we work to understand the science behind anxiety in young minds, we can use that science to develop treatments that are more effective.”
____________________
About The Author
Jill Suttie, Psy.D., is a free-lance journalist and a staff writer and contributing editor for Greater Good, an award-winning online magazine published by the University of California’s Greater Good Science Center. A psychologist by training, her articles cover scientific research aimed at uncovering the keys to individual wellbeing and a more compassionate society. She also records music and has two CD’s of original songs that can be heard and purchased on her personal website: jillsuttie.com.
____________________
To learn more about Kid Power camp, please visit these news items: